Mostra i principali dati dell'item

dc.contributor.authorMoliné, A.
dc.contributor.authorGálvez García, Germán
dc.contributor.authorFernández Gómez, J.
dc.contributor.authorde la Fuente, J.
dc.contributor.authorIborra, O.
dc.contributor.authorTornay, F.J.
dc.contributor.authorMata Martín, J.L.
dc.contributor.authorPuertollano, M.
dc.contributor.authorGómez Milán, E.
dc.date.accessioned2024-05-07T11:23:15Z
dc.date.available2024-05-07T11:23:15Z
dc.date.issued2017
dc.identifier.issn0048-5772
dc.identifier.urihttp://hdl.handle.net/10366/157705
dc.description.abstract[EN]We applied the use of thermography to cognitive neuropsychology, particularly as an objective marker of subjective experiences, in the context of lying. We conducted three experiments: (a) An important lie was invented by the participants in 3 min, and it was recounted by phone to a significant person while they were recorded by the thermographic camera, obtaining a face and hands map of the lie. (b) A similar methodology was carried out, but adding the Cold Stress Test (CST) of the dominant hand during the phone call, obtaining a second physiologic marker (the percentage of thermal recovery) to detect the lie. Further, it established a control condition where it generated anxiety in the participants using IAPS images with negative valence and high arousal, which were described by phone to a loved one. We obtained results that showed significant correlations between changes in body temperature and mental set. Of particular interest was the temperature of the nose and hand, which tended to decrease during lying (Experiment 1). The participants also showed a lower recovery of the temperature after the CST when they were lying (Experiment 2). (c) Experi ment 3 is a replication of Experiment 2 but with a different type of lie (a more ecological task) in a different scenario (following the ACID interview, with the use of the phone eliminated and participants motivated to lie well). The main pattern of results was replicated. We obtained an accuracy of 85% in detection of deception with 25% of false alarms.es_ES
dc.description.sponsorshipFunding information: CONICYT Chile (FONDECYT 1160368)es_ES
dc.format.mimetypeapplication/pdf
dc.language.isoenges_ES
dc.rightsAttribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 Internacional*
dc.rights.urihttp://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/*
dc.subjectCold Stress Testes_ES
dc.subjectDetection of deceptiones_ES
dc.subjectPinocchio effectes_ES
dc.subjectThermographyes_ES
dc.titleThe Pinocchio effect and the Cold Stress Test: Lies and thermographyes_ES
dc.typeinfo:eu-repo/semantics/articlees_ES
dc.relation.publishversionhttps://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/28714186/es_ES
dc.subject.unesco61 Psicologíaes_ES
dc.identifier.doi10.1111/psyp.12956
dc.rights.accessRightsinfo:eu-repo/semantics/openAccesses_ES
dc.identifier.essn1469-8986
dc.journal.titlePsychophysiologyes_ES
dc.volume.number54es_ES
dc.issue.number11es_ES
dc.page.initial1es_ES
dc.page.final11es_ES
dc.type.hasVersioninfo:eu-repo/semantics/publishedVersiones_ES


Files in questo item

Thumbnail

Questo item appare nelle seguenti collezioni

Mostra i principali dati dell'item

Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 Internacional
Excepto si se señala otra cosa, la licencia del ítem se describe como Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 Internacional